Term Paper on "Interview of Testing a Child's Ability"

Term Paper 6 pages (1740 words) Sources: 0

[EXCERPT] . . . .

Child Development

Piaget's Conservation and Childhood Justification

Piaget's theories of child development have been generalized and widely accepted across the domains of psychology, child development, and education. It would not be inappropriate to label him as one of the most significant thinkers in the modern practice of these fields. Yet his work is not unassailable. He based the majority of his observations on the study of his own children, and some critics have complained that he insufficiently explores the impact of cultural differences and the issue of giftedness among children. Intellectually gifted children may move through the stages differently, or perhaps with more self-awareness. (Cohen & Kim, 1999) Piaget's theories have remained under scrutiny, for these and other reasons, since their inception. To this day there is little agreement as to the precise accuracy of his stages or the way in which they may be measured and determined. Piaget himself measured the shift between stages by a child's ability to succeed in specific theory tasks which demonstrate certain cognitive skills, such as the ability to comprehend different points-of-view or the ability to conserve numbers, area, volume and so forth in relation to objects.

On the one hand, many developmental studies have utilized Piaget's theories and found them to be reliable. For example Uri Fidelman (1995) performed one study in which kindergarten children were tested for performance on the Piaget-designed stage task, asked to conserve numbers over varying spatial arrangements. At the same time they were tested for the development of right-brain or left-brain capability. Over the next
Continue scrolling to

download full paper
year, the children were tested again, and many of them were seen to shift stages and begin to conserve numbers. Those that underwent such a shift also experienced changes in the development of their brain hemispheres, with males and females becoming more alike at that point. This study, in addition to having interesting contributions to make to gender/sex research also seems to indicate a real, biological core to Piaget's theories. Of course, it could be argued that these changes in cognition and hemisphere capability were coincidental, both due to the child's maturing rather than directly linked in any way.

Arguments against a strict reliance on Piaget's stages, meanwhile, have amassed in the years since his introduction of the theory. The best of these arguments point out that though his stages may have some legitimacy as a general outline of childhood development, they vastly under-rate the actual capabilities of the majority of children by attempting to determine their developmental stage using tests which are inherently skewed against socially sensitive children. These studies argue that children do indeed think differently than adults, but that this difference has major factors which Piaget has not taken into consideration. The most commonly mentioned of these is the social factor. Children are socially dependent on adults for information and, perhaps more importantly, for what one might call permission. Adults consistently assert their power over children, with answers such as "because I said so" being considered legitimate logic to many parents and teachers. Children are expected to surrender their own opinions, even if they may be correct, in obeisance to adult opinion. Those that refuse to surrender their own opinions may be physically punished, socially humiliated or otherwise penalized, or even be treated with psychoactive drugs (defiance of authority is considered a primary symptom of "oppositional disorder"). Of course, children also depend entirely on adults for food, lodgings, and all the basic necessities of life -- as any feminist can point out, dependent groups tend to be discouraged from forcefully asserting their own views of the world.

Though most studies do not bring up the extremely serious issue of the way in which children are expected to surrender their opinions, they do point out that the young a child is the more hyper-sensitive they are to social atmosphere and subtle adult pressure. In the most basic structure of a Piaget test, adults ask children an obviously foolish question (such as "are these lines the same length?") and then after the child answers, they make some actually inconsequential change (such as relocating the line or spacing the elements differently) and then reiterate the question. As those of us who have ever been interrogated by an authority will recall, repetition of a question is socially understood even by adults to generally mean that the first answer was not good enough. Many studies have suggested that children may be subtly attempting to placate the adult by changing their first answer which the adult apparently thought was wrong. Even adults have been shown to change their reports of known facts in response to social pressure. Solomon Asch's infamous study (1955) showed that when adult male college students were asked to judge which lines out a selection of four lines were the same length (an easier task than the Piaget conservation of numbers of objects), they made 33% wrong identifications if all their comrades in the study gave the wrong answer. This isolation made them question their own perceptions. If marginal social pressure effects adult men, certainly one can expect more immediate social pressure -- that of having an adult question a child's perception -- would be enough to make a child give a wrong answer.

Of course, if children are just responding to subtle cues given by the interviewer, then one would expect studies to bear this out -- and they do. McGarrigle and Donaldson (1974) found that if Piaget stage tasks were performed in a way that minimalized the appearance that the first answer was wrong, the success rate of an average group of four-year-old children jumped from 22.5% to 80% -- a total reversal of the trend towards failure for that age. In this study, the researchers pretended that a "Naughty Teddy" toy escaped his cage and messed up the lines of objects. They researcher then asked for the child's help in making sure the lines were still had the same number of objects. Not only could 80% of the children succeed at this task, they could do so despite the fact that they could not be entirely sure that Naughty Teddy had not absconded with objects, placed new ones, or otherwise scrambled the contents of the two lines. Light (et al., 1979) critiqued Naughty Teddy for being a significant distraction from the task at hand, and suggested even higher numbers would have succeeded if the dancing bear hadn't distracted them. Light (et al., 1979) additionally showed that the famous liquid conservation study would show significantly higher rates of conservation if the researcher justified pouring the liquid from one vial to another by pointing out that the former beaker was chipped and had to be removed.

Judith Samuel and Peter Bryant (1984) found that a similar result could be achieved by asking only one question per task. They arranged a conservation test in which children were divided into three groups and presented with doh orbs that were subsequently squished into cylinders. (a traditional Piaget test). One group was presented with two questions in the traditional task format. The second group wasn't asked if the first set was equal, but were shown the balls being squashed and then asked if they were equal. The final group didn't even see the two balls, only one ball and one cylinder. The first group and the third had significantly poorer results than the second group, showing that children actually could carry over knowledge from seeing the ball, and that adult intervention would change their representation of that knowledge.

Another factor may be the issue of complex language being used for children whose language development has not developed as fully. (Children, like adults, take some time to learn language). Light (et al.) speaks of the importance of a child's lack of complex language (due to the fact that they have only had a few years to learn their native languages), and the way in which Piaget's complex wording could intentionally lead them astray. Markman and Seibert (1976), for example, that collection terms have a significant impact on how well children can process class information. For example, a traditional Piaget task would ask: "Who would have more to eat, someone who ate the green grapes or someone who ate the grapes?" And consider the right answer to be that the green grapes were included in the last category. Children, however, appeared to frequently assume that if the green grapes had been eaten, then only the other grapes would remain to be eaten, or that the researcher was somehow excluding the green grapes from the "grape" category. However, if the researcher asked "Who would have more to eat, someone who ate the green grapes or someone who ate the bunch?" then five and six-year-old were significantly more likely to have the right answer.

This former research calls into question Piaget's entire formula, suggesting that children may indeed think differently, but even more importantly they seem to approach adults, language, and situations differently. The research question which needs to be… READ MORE

Quoted Instructions for "Interview of Testing a Child's Ability" Assignment:

Course Subject: Child Development

Topic: Interview of Testing a Child's Ability (choose one of Piaget's theory tasks below)

Child Interview

Interview a child between the ages of 2-12. Be sure that the child is willing to participate and that the child's parents consent. Inform them that you will keep the interview confidential by using a pseudonym rather than the child's real name in your paper.

I. Select one of the two topics below related to child development that you are interested in exploring in greater depth.

- you can select one of Piaget's theory tasks when doing the interview to test child's level of ability:

-*****Conservation- test child's ability to distinguish appearence from reality depending on age

- Egocentrism- test child's ability to see if they are unable or able to take another person's perspective into account depending on their age

II. Find empirical references relevant to your topic

• Find TWO EMPIRICAL articles that are relevant to your topic (one article may come from the course readings).

One recommendation reference if you choose conservation:

-Flavell, J.H. (1986). The development of chidren's knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction, American Psychologist, 41(4), 418-425.

• Read these articles and formulate a hypothesis. Then, develop a set of interview questions designed to test your hypothesis.

III. Conduct the interview

• The interview should last for approximately 30-45 minutes

• Have your questions prepared in advance. You may take somewhat of a 'clinical interview' approach and ask follow-up questions based on your interviewee's responses, but you should have a set of general questions outlined prior to the interview.

• Find a quiet place to conduct the interview.

• During the interview take good notes (if your interviewee is willing, you may want to record the interview, but it is not a requirement).

• Inform the interviewee that he/she does not have to answer questions that he/she might find uncomfortable.

• Thank your interviewee for participating.

IV. Write up the paper

The body of your paper should be divided in to the following sections.

Introduction

• State why this is an important topic to study.

• Discuss the empirical studies in terms of their relevance to your topic. What does previous research show about your topic? Be sure to reference the empirical articles in your paper (use APA style referencing).

• Formally state your hypothesis (the empirical studies you discuss should provide a clear rationale for your hypothesis).

• Give a general overview of how your paper will be organized.

Method and Procedures

• Give a background description of your interviewee (age, gender, ethnicity, relationship to you)

• Describe your interview procedures

-describe the setting where you conducted the interview (private home recommended)

-offer a brief summary of your interview questions and testing materials (if any were used)

Results and Conclusions

• Present only the relevant data. DO NOT copy verbatim the entire interview.

• Discuss your interviewee’s responses. Were your results consistent with your hypothesis? Did your interviewee's responses differ from what you would have expected based on what you have learned in class or from what you have read in the empirical articles? Why do you think the results supported or failed to support your predictions?

• Offer a brief conclusion and discuss some of the implications of your findings.

***Paper Assignment Tips

Beyond meeting the basic assignment requirements, a good paper will be clearly written and carefully researched. So that you have a better chance of successfully writing up your interview, I have outlined below some suggestions about how to get your research started. These are primarily specific hints on how to form a hypothesis and operationally define your variables.

FORMING A HYPOTHESIS

A good hypothesis….

• has a strong rationale.

-Grounded in theory and/or the results of previous empirical studies

• is worded as a simple declarative sentence that clearly states your prediction.

Example:

- Hypothesis: "Boys will engage in rough and tumble play more frequently than girls."

- Research Question: "Are there gender differences in the frequency of rough and tumble play?"

-Research questions tend to be relatively broad and general. Hypotheses tend to be more narrow in scope- they specify the direction of your prediction so that your research question, in essence, becomes testable.

• is testable

- Once you narrow down your topic, you will need to think about the operational definition of your variables. For the purposes of this assignment that means you shouldn't select a topic that involves behaviors you can't measure using the interview method.

OPERATIONALLY DEFINING YOUR VARIABLES

Outlining an operational definition of the variables you are interested in studying is a crucial component of the research process. A clear description of how you will measure broad and abstract concepts like aggression or attachment is essential to the science of psychology because it allows us the ability to verify (test) our ideas. It also is crucial to the process of replication. Without a detailed description of your methods and procedures, others will be unable to test the reliability of your results.

1. Borrow your methods from previous empirical studies when appropriate.

After reading the empirical studies on your topic, think out how you can borrow and adapt the measures and procedures used by researchers. You don't have to use everything- only what is relevant to your specific hypothesis.

For the purposes of this assignment, probably the most simple and straightforward way for you to operationally define your variables will be to use measures and procedures that are already well-established in the literature (Piagetian tasks,). When you create measures of your own, you are never quite sure that you will successfully create a measure that is both valid and reliable.

Although it is not a requirement of the assignment, using well-established methods (and of course properly citing where these methods came from), will save you the headache of having to convince others that your new measures are sound. This is not a course in research methods, but a good paper has logical, valid, and reliable methods. Borrowing measures from other established researchers allows you relate your findings to the results of previous studies and it saves you from having to "re-invent the wheel".

You may, of course, decide to adapt the task to your particular project (e.g., using Oreos or Graham crackers instead of coins in a Piagetian conservation task), as long as the underlying logic of the task is the same.

How to Reference "Interview of Testing a Child's Ability" Term Paper in a Bibliography

Interview of Testing a Child's Ability.” A1-TermPaper.com, 2005, https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610. Accessed 6 Jul 2024.

Interview of Testing a Child's Ability (2005). Retrieved from https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610
A1-TermPaper.com. (2005). Interview of Testing a Child's Ability. [online] Available at: https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610 [Accessed 6 Jul, 2024].
”Interview of Testing a Child's Ability” 2005. A1-TermPaper.com. https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610.
”Interview of Testing a Child's Ability” A1-TermPaper.com, Last modified 2024. https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610.
[1] ”Interview of Testing a Child's Ability”, A1-TermPaper.com, 2005. [Online]. Available: https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610. [Accessed: 6-Jul-2024].
1. Interview of Testing a Child's Ability [Internet]. A1-TermPaper.com. 2005 [cited 6 July 2024]. Available from: https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610
1. Interview of Testing a Child's Ability. A1-TermPaper.com. https://www.a1-termpaper.com/topics/essay/child-development-piaget-conservation/34610. Published 2005. Accessed July 6, 2024.

Related Term Papers:

Children, Grief, and Attachment Theory Term Paper

Paper Icon

Children, Grief, And Attachment Theory

When a child, age 7 to 11, experiences the death of a nuclear or extended family member, the experi-ence generates subsequent grief reaction/s. During the… read more

Term Paper 75 pages (22384 words) Sources: 40 Topic: Child Development / Youth / Teens


Significant Connections for Families of an Intellectually Disabled Child Term Paper

Paper Icon

Disabled Scenario

Interview with Mrs. S., Special Education teacher in Moderntown Middle School, Anytown, USA.

When thinking about your students, what particular expertise do you believe one needs in order… read more

Term Paper 3 pages (1159 words) Sources: 1 Topic: Child Development / Youth / Teens


Negative Effects of Television on Children Research Paper

Paper Icon

Negative Effects of Television on Children

How television affects children? Research states that the average child watches television at least 2 hours per week and most of this viewing is… read more

Research Paper 10 pages (2867 words) Sources: 1+ Topic: Child Development / Youth / Teens


Testing vs. Alternative Forms of Assessment Term Paper

Paper Icon

Testing vs Alternative Forms of Assessment

Summary of "Testing vs. alternative forms of assessment"

Standardized tests, if used appropriately, can provide useful information about a non-native speaking student's skills and… read more

Term Paper 2 pages (688 words) Sources: 1+ Topic: Education / Teaching / Learning


Bulling and Academic Performance Term Paper

Paper Icon

Woodsa, S. & Wolkeb, D. "Direct and relational bullying among primary schoolchildren and academic achievement"

Article Review of Paper by Woodsa & Wolkeb

School bullying is a truly international issue;… read more

Term Paper 6 pages (1993 words) Sources: 0 Topic: Child Development / Youth / Teens


Sat, Jul 6, 2024

If you don't see the paper you need, we will write it for you!

Established in 1995
900,000 Orders Finished
100% Guaranteed Work
300 Words Per Page
Simple Ordering
100% Private & Secure

We can write a new, 100% unique paper!

Search Papers

Navigation

Do NOT follow this link or you will be banned from the site!